This article applies to:
E-Prime 3.0
E-Prime 1.0
Detail
Experiment Author: Susan Campbell. Adapted from STEP and used with permission of Brian MacWhinney
Experiment Description
In this task, participants were given a prime, followed by a target word that they were supposed to name. The primes were either homophones, pseudohomophones (nonwords that look like they should be pronounced like the target), or graphemic controls (words that looked like the target, but were not pronounced like it).
The homophones and pseudohomophones speeded naming the word, while graphemic controls seemed about as effective as normal (unrelated) controls. This implies that initial access to words is phonological, rather than graphemic.
Experiment Instructions
A line of hashtags "#####" appears followed by a priming word that appears for 30 milliseconds. The prime disappears and a target word appears. Participants say the target word out loud and press any key to move on to the next trial. There are 60 trials in the experiment.
NOTE: no .es3 version of this task is included due to compatibility issues with Windows 8 and 10. For more information, please see INFO: Windows 8 or DirectX 11 or greater detected [23888] and BUG: ClearAfter no longer working [30303].
Experiment Citation
Lukatela, G. and Turvey, M. T. (1994). Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 2. Evidence from phonological priming by homophones and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(4), 331-353.
Cited Experiment Abstract
Seven experiments were conducted that examined phonological and orthographic priming of naming using three- and four-field masking procedures with prolonged targets. Experiments 1-3 found significant phonological priming by homophones (TOWED-toad) that was independent of prime identifiability and prime-target stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA: 30, 60, or 250ms). Subsequent experiments found significant phonological priming by pseudohomophones (TODE-toad) that was similarly independent of prime identifiability and SOA. Collectively, the limited effects of orthographic control primes (TOLD-toad, TODS-toad) and the pronounced and orthographically independent effects of phonological primes suggest (a) a leading role in visual word perception for a fast-acting, automatic, assembled phonology, and (b) a phonological basis, rather than an abstract graphemic basis, for the processing equivalency of letter variations.
Works Cited by the Experiment
Aaronson, D., & Ferres, S. (1983). A model for coding lexical categories during reading.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 700-725.
Carello, C., Turvey, M.T., & Lukatela, G. (1992). Can theories of word recognition remain stubbornly nonphonological? In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 211-226). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Chappell, M., & Humphreys, M.S. (1994). An auto-associative neural network for sparse representations: Analysis and application to models of recognition and cued recall. Psychological Review, 101, 103-128.
Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151-216). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Colombo, L. (1986). Activation and inhibition with orthographically similar words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 226-234.
Davelaar, E., Coltheart, M., Besner, D., & Jonasson, J.T. (1978). Phonological recoding and lexical access. Memory & Cognition, 6, 391-402.
Evett, L.J., & Humphreys, G.W. (1981). The use of abstract graphemic information in lexical access. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 325-350.
Fehrer, E., & Biederman, I. (1962). A comparison of reaction time and verbal report in the detection of masked stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 126-130.
Fehrer, E., & Raab, D. (1962). Reaction time to stimuli masked by metacontrast. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 143-147.
Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1992). Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Evidence from masked nonword priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 353-372.
Fleming, K.K. (1993). Phonologically mediated priming in spoken and printed word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 272-284.
Forster, K. (1987). Form-priming with masked primes: The best match hypothesis. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. XII, pp. 127-146). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Forster, K., & Davis, C. (1991). The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 1-25.
Forster, K., Davis, C., Schnoknecht, C., & Carter, R. (1987). Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition of partial activation? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 211-251.
Grainger, J. (1992). Orthographic neighborhoods and visual word recognition. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 131-146). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Grossberg, S. (1980). How does a brain build a cognitive code? Psychological Review, 87, 1-51.
Grossberg, S., & Stone, G.O. (1986). Neural dynamics of word recognition and recall: Priming, learning, and resonance. Psychological Review, 93, 46-74.
Hanson, V.L., & Fowler, C.A. (1987). Phonological coding in word reading: Evidence from hearing and deaf readers. Memory & Cognition, 15, 199-207.
Hillinger, M.L. (1980). Priming effects with phonemically similar words: The encoding bias hypothesis reconsidered. Memory & Cognition, 8, 115-123.
Hinton, G.E., & Shallice, T. (1991). Lesioning an attractor network: Investigations of acquired dyslexia. Psychological Review, 98, 74-95.
Humphreys, G.W., Besner, D., & Quinlan, P.T. (1988). Event perception and the word repetition effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 51-67.
Humphreys, G.W., Evett, L.J., & Taylor, D.E. (1982). Automatic phonological priming in visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 10, 576-590.
Humphreys, G.W., & Evett, L.J. (1985). Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 689-740.
Humphreys, G.W., Evett, L.J., Quinlan, P.T., & Besner, D. (1987). Orthographic priming: Qualitative differences between priming from identified and unidentified primes. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. XII, pp. 105-125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kolers, P. (1970). Three stages of reading. In H. Levin & J.P. Williams (Eds.), Basic studies on reading (pp. 90-118). New York: Basic Books.
Kucera, H., & Francis, W.N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.
Lesch, M.F., & Pollatsek, A. (1993). Automatic access of semantic information by phonological codes in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 285-294.
Lukatela, G., Carello, C., & Turvey, M.T. (1990a). Phonemic priming by words and pseudowords. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2, 375-394.
Lukatela, G., Carello, C., & Turvey, M.T. (1990b). Phonemic, associative, and grammatical context effects with identified and unidentified primes. Language and Speech, 33, 1-18.
Lukatela, G., Lukatela, K., Carello, C., & Turvey, M.T. (1993). Delay and degradation reduce alphabet priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.
Lukatela, G., Lukatela, K., & Turvey, M.T. (1993). Further evidence for phonological constraints on visual lexical access: TOWED primes FROG. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 461-466.
Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M.T. (1990a). Phonemic similarity effect and prelexical phonology. Memory & Cognition, 18, 128-152.
Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M.T. (1990b). Automatic and prelexical computation of phonology in visual word identification. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2, 325-343.
Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M.T. (1991). Phonological access of the lexicon: Evidence from associative priming with pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 951-966.
Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M.T. (1993). Similar attentional, frequency, and associative effects for pseudohomophones and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 166-178.
Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M.T. (1994). Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 1. Evidence from associative priming by words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 107-128.
Lukatela, G., Turvey, M.T., Feldman, L.B., Carello, C., & Katz, L. (1989). Alphabetic priming in bi-alphabetical word perception. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 237-254.
Lukatela, G., Turvey, M.T., Todorovic, D. (1991). Is alphabet biasing in bialphabetical word perception automatic and prelexical? Journal of Expeirmental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 653-663.
Martin, R.C., & Jensen, C.R. (1988). Phonological priming in the lexical decision task: Failure to replicate. Memory & Cognition, 16, 505-521.
McClelland, J.L, & Rumelhart, D.E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375-407.
Michaels, C., & Turvey, M.T. (1979). Central sources of visual masking: Indexing structures supporting seeing at a single brief glance. Psychological Research, 41, 1-61.
Meyer, D.E., Schvaneveldt, R.W., & Ruddy, M.G. (1974). Functions of graphemic and phonemic codes in visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 2, 309-321.
Neely, J.H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264-336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Paap, K.R., Newsome, S.L., McDonald, J.E., & Schvaneveldt, R.W. (1982). An activation-verification model for letter and word recognition: The word superiority effect. Psychological Review, 89, 573-594.
Paap, K.R., & Noel, R.W. (1991). Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race. Psychological Research, 53, 13-24.
Paap, K.R., Noel, R.W., & Johansen, L.S. (1992). Dual-route models of print to sound: Red herrings and real horses. In R. Frost and L.Katz (Eds.), Orthography, morphology, and meaning (pp. 293-318). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Perfetti, C.A., & Bell, L.C. (1991). Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and masked priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 473-485.
Perfetti,C.A., Bell, L.C., & Delaney, S. (1988). Automatic (prelexical) phonetic activation in silent word reading: Evidence from backward masking. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 59-70.
Perffetti, C.A., Zhang, S., & Berent, I. (1992). Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a "universal" phonological principle. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 227-248). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Peter, M., Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M.T. (1990). Phonological priming: Failure to replicate in the rapid naming task. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 389-392.
Peter, M., & Turvey, M.T. (1994). Phonological codes are early sources of constraint on visual semantic categorization. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 497-504.
Schulman, H., Hornak, R., & Sanders, E. (1978). The effect of graphemic, phonetic, and semantic relationships on access to lexical structures. Memory & Cognition, 6, 115-123.
Seidenberg, M.S., & McClelland, J.L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523-568.
Smith, F. (1971). Understanding reading: A psycholinguistic analysis of reading and learning to read. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Smolensky, P. (1986). Formal modeling of subsymbolic processes: An introduction to harmony theory. In N.E. Sharkey (Ed.), Advances in cognitive science (Vol. 1, pp. 204-235). New York: Wiley.
Turvey, M.T. (1978). Visual processing and short-term memory. In W.K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 5, pp. 91-140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Van Orden, G.C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15, 181-198.
Van Orden, G.C., & Goldinger, S.D. (1994). The interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20.
Van Orden, G.C., Johnston, J.C., & Hale, B.L. (1988). Word identification in reading progresses from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 371-385.
Van Orden, G.C., Pennington, B.F., & Stone, G.O. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. Psychological Review, 97, 488-522.
Van Orden, G.C., Stone, G.O., Garlington, K., Markson, L., Pinnt, G., Simonfy, C., & Brichetto, A. (1992). "Assembled" phonology and reading: A case study in how theoretical perspective shapes empirical investigation. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, morphology, and meaning (pp. 249-290). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Wiles, J., & Humphreys, M.S. (1993). Using artificial neural nets to model implicit and explicit memory test performance. In P. Graf, & M.E.J. Messon (Eds.), Implicit memory: New directions in cognition, development, and neuropsychology (pp. 141-165). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wiles, J., Humphreys, M.S., Bain, J.D., & Dennis, S. (1991). Direct memory access using two cues: Finding the intersection of two sets in a connectionist model. In R.p. Lippman, J.E. Moody, & D. Touretzky (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems (Vol. 3, pp. 635-641). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
See Also:
STEP: Visual Lexical Analysis is Initially Phonological: 1 [35236]
STEP: Automatic (prelexical) Phonetic Activation in Silent Word Reading [35354]
Comments
0 comments
Please sign in to leave a comment.